Tuesday, September 18, 2012

2012 09 18 Historically Important Studies Pt. 1


Flower, L., & Hayes, J. R. (1981). A cognitive process theory of writing. College Composition and Communication, 32(4). 365-387.

I will work on the citation for this printing in Cross-Talk in Comp Theory. What version are the pdfs from?

Author: Linda S. Flower: lf54@andrew.cmu.edu BA – English and French, Simpson College ‘65; PhD – English, Rutgers ’72. Professor of Rhetoric, Dept of English at Cernegie Mellon University. Early work in cognitive processes in writing, looking to problem-solve in instruction. More recent focus on constructing negotiated meaning within competing identities and cultures. Looking at new literacy and inner city, multi-cultural communities, she is researching “intercultural rhetoric and education for community consequences.” http://english.cmu.edu/people/faculty/homepages/flower/default.html

John R. Hayes: jh50@andrew.cmu.edu Also at CMU, in the Psychology Department, funds the John R. Hayes Award for excellence in writing research ($1000) published in Journal of Writing Research. Pioneer in cognitive psychology within writing research – HEAVILY cited. Necessary to read for background on Authors’ Study. Looks at expert vs. novice; processes of planning, composing (even down to sentence level), and revision; the role of knowledge base for creative masters.

Date: Early 1980s – process model (exposure to broad content – encouraging process that lead to competency growth) is different than heritage model (transmitting culture and values with a cannon of study) and competencies model (producing mastery in hierarch of skills in sequence). Donald Graves, Lucy M. Calkins, and Nancy Atwell are big names in Process.

Research Questions: What guides the decisions writers make as they write? Creation of Cognitive Process Theory:
Flower_Hayes_Cognitive_Process_Theory_4_principles_p_366.png
Goals or even establishing entirely new ones based on what has been learned in the act of writing.

Context (need for study): Talk of the composition process of choice making needs to be backed up with answers. Kinneavy says, “Informing persuading, expressing, or manipulating language for its own sake” while Moffett and Gibson say, “Sense of the relation of speakers, subject, and audience” (Odell, Cooper, and Courts) (p. 365). Bitzer = response to a rhetorical situation.; Vatz = all response and situation “are determained by the imagination and art of the speaker” (p. 366). James Britton = process led by syntactic and lexical choices.

Methods: First: defining sub-process categories, show how these elements interact, want model to speak to what has remained unseen. Protocol analysis – not introspective, used in other cognitive process studies. Participants were to write an article on their job for Seventeen magazine, prepping as normal, but “composing out loud near an unobtrusive tape recorder” (p. 370). Transcript, together with manuscript, details writer’s process.

Findings: 370 – “The act of writing involves three major elements which are reflected in the three units of the model: the task environment, the writer’s long-term memory, and the writing process.” Writing is a process of setting goals, testing/reevaluating them, and consolidating/regenerating into new ones, often more complex. The writer is the creator, the decision maker, the one working – rather than being manipulated by forces.

Flower_Hayes_Structure_of_the_Writing_Model.png

Discussion (my connection): Moffett and Gibson seem to be getting at my general leaning for what constitutes voice. I’m pretty sure I’ve heard Lloyd Bitzer’s “rhetorictcal situation, which he succinctly defines as containing an exigency (which demands a response), an audience, and a set of constraints”  (pp. 365-266) in my program at Utah State. As far as using a protocol analysis since it has been used in other cognitive studies I feel that you are going to find what you want when you look through a lens that leads you there. Perhaps it is the bent of the psychologist who can speak the language of composition. The writer is no longer on a conveyor belt, being pushed along by external forces although a timed writing sample to be created in front of a computer you may or may not know how to use can seem forceful. Students often see the process of writing as a three-step process, which gives the impression of final drafts really being final. It is tough now with instant publication on the Internet, but the process for writers to get a published piece out takes much more time than many give to school assignments. I am intrigued by the protocol analysis, hearing someone talk out their decisions while also getting their manuscript. I do not think, though, that I could be the subject and not go into introspection during the session.


Perl, S. (1990). Understanding composing. In T. Newkirk (Ed.), To compose (41-51). Portsmouth, NH: Heineman.

Author: Sondra Perl – English Department at Lehman College. She lists academic interests in writing, teaching, creative nonfiction, ethnography, women's studies, holocaust studies, cross-cultural dialogue, urban education, collaborative projects, writing across the curriculum (ought to be similar or informative for Nancy Sommers’ book about the Harvard students). http://www.lehman.edu/academics/arts-humanities/english/faculty-perl.php She looks at attitudes, processes, and teaching of writing.

Date: 1990s – movement toward greater accountability in field of education. NCTE and IRA joined in developing a common set of national standards for ELA (viewing, visually representing, reading, writing, listening, and speaking) – specific outcomes students should be able to do. Demographics are changing and linguistic and cultural diversities are growing in school populations.

Research Questions: To what do writers move back? What exactly is being repeated? What recurs?

Research in class brought teachers to questions of: What basic patterns seem to occur during composing? What does this type of research have to tell us about he nature of the composing process?

Context (need for study): Emig (1971), Flowers and Hayes (1980), and Sommers (1979) are among those questioning the traditional and linear model of writing (plan-write-revise).

Methods: Group of 20 teachers in research and basic writing at NYU in 1979, all tape recording their thoughts while composing aloud in “My Most Anxious Moment as a Writer”. Understanding of controlling.altering  process with assigned topic and asking to compose aloud. Protocol analysis? On page 45, it seems the observations come from a career of observations, not just the moment at NYU.

Findings: “Recognition of recursiveness in writing” (p. 44). Writing is recursive, but the parts that recur vary from writer to writer. 1) Writers reread chunks 2) Returning to key word or notion of the topic 3) Movement to feelings and perceptions that the already present text evokes in the writer. “Those who realize that writing can be a recursive process have an easier time with waiting, looking, and discovering. Those who subscribe to the linear model find themselves easily frustrated when what they write does not immediately correspond to what they planned or when what they produce leaves them with little sense of accomplishment” (pp. 49-50). Projective structuring puts the writer into the place of the reader, imagining what other readers could need from the piece – to do so requires one to have experience as a reader. Other focus on process (turned into finding and doing what the teacher wants) and ignore their felt sense and do not connect to the writing.

Discussion (my connection): I wonder, since this study is done with teachers and the Flowers and Hayes group used professionals to describe their job, what the writing process looks like for students of all ages, but I think quickly to my students at Sweet Home High School – not the ones who would write along the lines of what I asked, but the ones who were dragging their feet, refusing, or providing me with minimal product. The environment and instruction I presented were not ideal nor would I want to reenact what I have done, but I assume that this is a very real place for teachers to confront the person who does not live (in school) “like a writer”. Are there similar studies with fly-under-the-radar-type students? Have these students made it through school without having experienced the momentum-high of creating through writing?  I involve myself with the felt sense often. I move my hands, say some words over and over, [doing it right now], in order to grasp a sense of my own understanding before, not through, my fingers on the keys. Even projective structuring that calls me into the reader role requires me to return to my felt sense of being a reader.


Sommers, N. (1980). Revision strategies of student writers and experienced adult writers. College Composition and Communication, 31(4), 378-388.

I will work on the citation for this printing in Cross-Talk in Comp Theory. What version are the pdfs from?

Author: Nancy Sommers: nancy_sommers@gse.harvard.edu; Adjunct Lecturer on Education at Harvard. Ed.D from Boston University.  Interested in writing development and literacy skills of college and high school students – tracking 400 students from Harvard class of ’01 to understand role of writing in undergraduate education.  Also look for “Responding to Student Writing” (I’ve read it…I think with Brock Dethier). http://www.gse.harvard.edu/directory/faculty/faculty-detail/?fc=82044&flt=s&sub=all

Date: Early 1980s – process model (exposure to broad content – encouraging process that lead to competency growth) is different than heritage model (transmitting culture and values with a cannon of study) and competencies model (producing mastery in hierarch of skills in sequence). Donald Graves, Lucy M. Calkins, and Nancy Atwell are big names in Process.

Research Questions: What roles does revision play in the writing processes of college student writers and experienced (professional) writers?

Context (need for study): Absent research on revision within the writing process since current (1980) models are mostly linear and move away from revision (inner speech – meanings of words – in words…revision is not there).

Methods: Case study – 20 freshmen at Boston University and the University of Oklahoma with SAT verbals between 450-600 while in their first semester of college composition. 20 experienced, adult writers from Boston and OKC (journalists, editors, academics). Series of studies over 3 years. Each writer wrote an expressive, explanatory, and persuasive essay. All three essays were rewritten twice resulting in nine drafts or final drafts. Interviews and suggestions for other authors three times after each final revision. Essays were analyzed and changes were categorized. Interview transcripts were used to create a scale of concerns that each writer had.

Findings: Revision is “a sequence of changes in a composition—changes which are initiated by cures and occur continually throughout the writing of a work.” Four revision operations: deletion, substitution, addition, and reordering. Four levels of change: word, phrase, sentence, theme (the extended statement of one idea). Students hardly used the terms “revision” or “rewriting,” instead using alternate definitions and see this step as changing words – hardly meaning – in order to clean up speech. Their text was solid and existed in its entirety but needed to be communicated better. They see no need to revise if one can read and not get tripped up. Changes in ideas came in modifying an introductory paragraph, as if the hook was all that was needed. They lack the procedures or experience to reason through questions of purposes and readers. Experienced writers are rewriting to find “the form or shape of their argument” in patterns, frameworks, or designs. The initial writing is done to find what to say. Their second drafts are looking toward structure. Revisions are necessary when they recognize a disconnect between intention and execution. Experienced writers change mostly on the sentence level, but they change on all levels – unlike the students.

Discussion (my connection): Sommers notes that linear models are based off of classical rhetoric for oratory – revision isn’t existent in speech. I need to be thinking about structure origins. I am reminded of the letter that Joey writes in support of Monica and Chandler as adoptive parents in an episode of Friends when he used a thesaurus on every word he could, signing the letter “Baby kangaroo Tribiani”. In order to recognize your own incongruities, you have to be a critical reader, as Perl states the necessity to be an experienced reader. It takes more time than students generally give, because writing is only a way to get through to the grade for many – sweeping generalization. Similar to the felt sense, “at the heart of revision is the process by which writers recognize and resolve the dissonance they sense in their writing” (p 51). At issue is maturation. The child smashes more trash into the trash can. The father takes the trash out. Students respond to what they are taught - that writing is linear and are simply doing what comes next in order to clean up appearances. The experienced writers have taken the risks to discover through reading and writing and to look for problems in their own writing. Students have a sense of writing to placate, not to disturb.

No comments:

Post a Comment